EssaysForStudent.com - Free Essays, Term Papers & Book Notes
Search

The Multiplicity Problem

By:   •  Research Paper  •  3,346 Words  •  May 6, 2010  •  873 Views

Page 1 of 14

The Multiplicity Problem

THE MULTIPLICITY PROBLEM

Everyday, one can find, in the newspaper or in other popular press, some

claim of association between a stimulus and an outcome, with

consequences for health or general welfare of the population at large.

Many of these associations are suspect at best, and often will not hold

up under scrutiny. Examples of such association are: coffee and heart

attacks, vitamins and IQ, tomato sauce and cancer, and on and on. Many

of these claims have shaky foundations, and some have not been

replicated in further research. With so much conflicting information in

the popular press, the general public has learned to mistrust

statistical studies, and to shy away from the use of statistics in general.

There are several reasons that cause these incorrect conclusions to

become part of the scientific and popular press; usually scientists

fault such things as improper study design and poor data. Another reason

for these claims originates from large studies, where data analysts

report all the tests that are "statistically significant" (usually

defined as p < 0.05, where "p" denotes p-value) as a "real" effect. On

the surface, this practice seems innocuousness; since this is the rule

learned in statistics classes. The problem arises when multiple test are

performed "p < 0.05" outcome can often occur when there is no real

effect at all. Historically, the "p < 0.05" rule was devised for a

single test with the following logic: if p < 0.05 outcome was observed,

than the analyst has two options Either heshe can believe that there is

no real effect and that the data is so anomalous that it is within the

range of values that would be observed only 1 in 20, or he/she may

choose to believe that the observed association is real. Because the 1

in 20 chance is relatively small, the common practice is to "reject" the

hypothesis of no real effect and "accept" the conclusion that the effect

is real.

The logic breaks down when more than one test and comparisons are

considered in a single study. If one considers 20 or more tests, than

one expects at least one "1 in 20" significant outcome, even when none

of the effects are real. Thus, there is little protection offered by the

"1 in 20" rule, and incorrect claims can result.

Although incorrect decisions can be blamed on poor design, bad data,

etc., one should be aware that multiplicity can cause faulty

conclusions, and should be taken care off in large studies that includes

many tests and comparisons.

One example for these kinds of studies is: subgroup analysis in a

clinical trial.

As a part of the pharmaceutical development process new therapies

usually are evaluated using randomized clinical trials. In such studies,

patients are randomly assigned to either active or placebo therapy.

After the conclusion of the study, the active or placebo groups are

compared to see which is better, using a single pre-defined outcome of

interest. At this stage, there is no multiplicity problem, since there

is only one test. However, there are many reasons to evaluate patient

subgroups. The therapy might

Download as (for upgraded members)  txt (24.3 Kb)   pdf (323.5 Kb)   docx (25.8 Kb)  
Continue for 13 more pages »